59 pages • 1 hour read
A modern alternative to SparkNotes and CliffsNotes, SuperSummary offers high-quality Study Guides with detailed chapter summaries and analysis of major themes, characters, and more.
In the brief introduction to Part 2, Bregman grapples with a critical question: if humans are innately good, how can atrocities like Auschwitz be explained? This dilemma not only haunted the author but also made German publishers wary. Bregman rejects the idea that those who commit such acts are fundamentally different, noting that the Holocaust occurred in advanced, cultured Germany.
In response to the Holocaust, social psychology emerged in the 1950s and 1960s to study human behavior in extreme conditions. Key experiments by Stanley Milgram, Muzafer Sherif, and Philip Zimbardo examined how situational factors could turn ordinary people into perpetrators of cruelty. Bregman sets the stage for a deeper look into these influential studies, suggesting that their archives now offer an opportunity for better informed review.
Bregman offers a critical reevaluation of the infamous Stanford Prison Experiment, originally conducted by Philip Zimbardo in 1971. The experiment, aimed at investigating the psychological impact of perceived power, quickly descended into chaos. Participants assigned the role of prison guards engaged in sadistic behavior, leading to emotional distress among those playing prisoners. Zimbardo himself became so consumed by his supervisory role that the experiment had to be terminated prematurely. The chapter leans heavily on personal anecdotes and observations, questioning the innate morality or immorality of humans when exposed to environmental stimuli.
Bregman introduces revelations that fundamentally challenge the integrity of Zimbardo’s study. Drawing on the investigative work of French sociologist Thibault Le Texier, the chapter unmasks the premeditated and manipulated nature of the experiment. Guards were explicitly coached to create a sense of powerlessness among prisoners, fundamentally altering the interpretation of their actions from spontaneous cruelty to coerced behavior. This unveiling introduces the concept of “demand characteristics,” wherein subjects modify their actions to align with the researchers’ expectations, thereby compromising the study’s objectivity.
Additionally, Bregman discusses another significant but less sensational study by British psychologists Alexander Haslam and Stephen Reicher. Designed as a contrast to Zimbardo’s experiment and sponsored by the BBC, this study left the participants’ actions uninfluenced by the researchers. Contrary to Zimbardo’s results, the guards and prisoners in this study exhibited empathy and even formed a commune. Despite the absence of compelling drama, Haslam and Reicher considered their study a success, publishing multiple academic articles that suggest human behavior is not universally corruptible under the guise of authority.
The chapter thus serves as a dissection of two parallel but fundamentally different studies, both aiming to scrutinize the core elements of human nature under situational influences. While Zimbardo’s experiment has attained iconic status, the questions raised about its validity and ethical considerations are deeply unsettling. In comparison, the British study, although less captivating for mass audiences, presents a contrasting and potentially more realistic interpretation of human decency. These examples, in turn, challenge long-standing assumptions about the corrupting influence of power.
Chapter 8 takes on complex questions about authority, obedience, and human nature. Centered around Stanley Milgram’s obedience experiments from the 1960s, the chapter first presents Milgram’s shocking findings that suggested most people would harm another under authoritative pressure. Milgram tied his results to larger atrocities, like the Holocaust, lending academic weight to Hannah Arendt’s concept of the “banality of evil”—the notion that ordinary people can commit horrifying acts under specific conditions. This seminal research became an accepted explanation for why people commit malicious acts, supporting Arendt’s analysis of Nazi war criminal Adolf Eichmann, whom she portrayed as an unthinking bureaucrat following orders.
However, the chapter uncovers inconsistencies and ethical concerns in Milgram’s work, spurred by researcher Gina Perry and others, who found Milgram had manipulated results to fit his thesis. This revelation invites a reevaluation of the banality of evil, opening a conversation about the ethics and integrity of psychological research itself. Psychologists Alex Haslam and Steve Reicher introduce an alternative explanation—that participants were identifying with the authority figure rather than mindlessly obeying them. This potential explanation raises the possibility that individuals may commit harmful acts because they believe they are contributing to a greater good. This theory reframes the conclusions from not only Milgram’s experiment but also the infamous Stanford Prison Experiment, offering the nuanced view that “evil” acts are often carried out under the guise of doing “good.”
Bregman pushes the reader further by exploring Adolf Eichmann’s trial, revealing that the man was not simply an obedient servant but a fervent ideologue. This finding supports the argument that humans, rather than being mere obedient automatons, are beings driven by a perverse sense of righteousness. This twist in understanding adds complexity to Arendt’s “banality of evil,” suggesting that malevolent actions may derive from a twisted sense of doing “good” rather from than blind obedience to authority.
Finally, Bregman offers a counterpoint to the inherently evil human nature hypothesis through the historical example of the Danish resistance against Nazi persecution. Contrary to Milgram’s assertion, the collective actions of the Danes demonstrate that people can, indeed, choose compassion and resistance over evil when morally engaged. By integrating psychological studies and historical case studies, Bregman challenges the reader to question conventional wisdom about human nature. The chapter argues that an understanding of inherent human goodness not only exists but also imposes a moral imperative to act against evil.
Bregman turns to exploring human behavior and social responsibility, using case studies and data to challenge prevailing assumptions. The chapter opens with the story of Kitty Genovese, a young woman who was murdered outside her New York City apartment in 1964. Despite her screams, no one came to her aid. This event was initially framed by the media as evidence of societal decay and moral apathy, giving rise to the psychological construct known as the “bystander effect.” Research by psychologists Bibb Latané and John Darley provided scientific validation, with the researchers going so far as to posit that the more people who witness an emergency, the less likely anyone is to intervene, as each person assumes someone else will act.
The narrative pivots to another incident that occurred in Amsterdam in 2016, when a woman named Sanne and her toddler were rescued from a submerged car by immediate bystander intervention. The chapter pairs this event with discussion of a 2011 meta-analysis that reconsiders the bystander effect, suggesting that in life-threatening emergencies, bystanders often do intervene, especially if they can communicate with each other. Danish psychologist Marie Lindegaard’s real-world video analyses reinforce this finding, indicating that people help each other in 90% of observed emergencies.
As the chapter concludes, a fresh perspective on the Kitty Genovese case emerges through the meticulous research of amateur historian Joseph De May. His findings reveal that the media narrative was sensationalized and inaccurate. At least two calls were made to the police, and Kitty died in the arms of a neighbor, Sophia Farrar, who rushed to her aid. This reality conflicts with the popular depiction of the incident as a grim testimony of public apathy, a depiction nurtured by the media and even psychological studies.
Bregman calls for a reevaluation of widely accepted narratives and assumptions. Rather than relying on sensationalized accounts, he promotes a nuanced understanding. Human nature is complex, but it leans more toward compassion and community than commonly believed. From the cautionary tale of Kitty Genovese to the uplifting story of Sanne, the chapter stresses the importance of both action and inaction as indicators of societal values, advocating that understanding the intricacies of human behavior requires debunking myths and reassessing preconceptions.
Part 2 relies heavily on Bregman’s scrutiny of journalism, seminal research, and the accepted norms of social psychology. His efforts to deconstruct and critique major studies and widely accepted myths prompt readers to question the foundations of their broader understanding of human nature.
For example, Part 2 tackles the pressing existential question of how Bregman reconciles his belief in the Innate Goodness of Humans with the atrocities of Auschwitz and the Holocaust more broadly. Bregman confronts this dilemma by revisiting classic social psychology experiments like those of Stanley Milgram, Philip Zimbardo, and Muzafer Sherif. These experiments, carried out in the wake of the Holocaust, sought to shed light on the “dark side” of humanity by exploring how situational factors could make ordinary people capable of cruel actions. However, by examining the archives and critiquing the methodologies of these landmark studies, Bregman contests their conclusions.
Bregman employs literary and rhetorical elements to navigate this ethical minefield, with an emphasis in Part 2 on highlighting The Impact of Societal Structures on Human Behavior. Utilizing investigative journalism, he incorporates personal anecdotes and scientific research into his arguments. For example, he draws on the work of French sociologist Thibault Le Texier to challenge the validity of Zimbardo’s Stanford Prison Experiment, which has long been held up as a standard example of how power can corrupt. Bregman dissects the contrived circumstances of this experiment to show how Zimbardo’s guards were prompted into their sadistic behaviors, thus nullifying the experiment as a genuine investigation into human nature. This methodical questioning of the status quo carries over into Bregman’s analysis of Milgram’s obedience experiments. As with Zimbardo’s work, Bregman digs into the archives and presents evidence that Milgram manipulated the experiment’s results to fit his thesis. The exposure of such manipulations calls into question not only the validity of these studies but also the ethical considerations of such research. This detailed scrutiny of primary sources allows Bregman to call into question, and often debunk, long-standing conclusions about the banality of evil and the corrupting influence of authority.
Bregman also introduces alternative perspectives to the dialogue, such as the experiments by British psychologists Alexander Haslam and Stephen Reicher, which again bring nuance to his picture of human behavior. Contrary to Zimbardo’s findings, their research shows that when not prompted by authority, individuals do not naturally devolve into tyrants but can express empathy and cooperation. In emphasizing The Importance and Prevalence of Cooperation, Bregman notes as well historical cases like the Danish resistance during the Holocaust. Such examples demonstrate how the collective actions of a society can manifest moral courage rather than moral decline.
At the heart of Bregman’s narrative is a critique of how societal structures and cultural narratives shape human behavior. The author draws attention to the power of media, especially in the perpetuation of the myth of the “bystander effect,” through the distorted lens of the Kitty Genovese case. He discusses this flawed portrayal of a real-world event in parallel with more recent examples and research data that indicate humans are more likely to help than to ignore others in distress. This discussion is a punctilious rebuttal of sensationalized media narratives, supporting Bregman’s broader claim that media has skewed people’s understanding of human nature.
Bregman’s methodology of questioning the accepted narrative by revisiting primary sources and including alternative research provides a variety of perspectives that lead to a reevaluation of human nature. Here is where the essence of his argument lies: Humans are not mere products of their environment, capable of evil under certain conditions, but beings inherently inclined toward goodness and cooperation. Such a proposition not only challenges people’s intellectual conceptions but also has profound implications for how humans shape their societies and systems of governance. In probing the workings of human psychology and social constructs, Humankind thus serves as a call to action: Humans must not only reassess and reevaluate but also act with a renewed sense of moral urgency.
Plus, gain access to 8,800+ more expert-written Study Guides.
Including features:
Anthropology
View Collection
Community
View Collection
Good & Evil
View Collection
New York Times Best Sellers
View Collection
Philosophy, Logic, & Ethics
View Collection
Politics & Government
View Collection
Popular Book Club Picks
View Collection
Popular Study Guides
View Collection
Psychology
View Collection
Science & Nature
View Collection
Sociology
View Collection